book:positive_computing:9_mindfulness
Differences
This shows you the differences between two versions of the page.
Both sides previous revisionPrevious revisionNext revision | Previous revision | ||
book:positive_computing:9_mindfulness [2016/07/11 22:47] – ↷ Page moved from hyo:private:positive_computing:9_mindfulness to book:positive_computing:9_mindfulness hkimscil | book:positive_computing:9_mindfulness [2016/07/12 12:07] (current) – hkimscil | ||
---|---|---|---|
Line 1: | Line 1: | ||
- | |<WRAP> | + | . |
- | < | + | . |
- | Mindfulness | + | . |
- | <imgcaption image1|Adele Krusche | + | . |
- | Adele Krusche and J. Mark G. Willaims, Oxford Mindfulness Centre, University of Oxford | + | . |
- | < | + | . |
- | Computing is not about computers any more. It is about living. | + | |
- | < | + | |< 100% 50% 50% >| |
+ | | < | ||
+ | ====== 9 Mindfulness | ||
+ | <blockquote> | ||
</ | </ | ||
- | At the Oxford Mindfulness Centre, we teach people the skills to be able to let go of unwanted thoughts, to sit still and to pay attention to what is going on right here and now. Within Oxford University’s Department of Psychiatry, we work with partners around the world to prevent depression and enhance human potential through the therapeutic use of mindfulness. The benefits of mindfulness in preventing serious depression and emotional distress have been demonstrated by clinical trials, and work continues to explore the implications of this relationship and to extend the approach to other disorders, using brain-imaging techniques together with experimental cognitive science to learn how mindfulness has its effects and which practices are best for whom. | ||
- | The society we live in and mindfulness therapy | + | You get dressed, spill some coffee, put the cereal away in the fridge, pretend you're listening to your kids, and leave without the car keys. It's a typical day in the world of the modern Homo sapiens -- a species that has largely lost its natural state of present awareness. We live on autopilot, lost in plans and reruns. Our attention flies off like a coven of witches to all the things we have to do, reliving the conversations we've had or rehearsing the ones we haven' |
- | The problem comes when we have classes on mindfulness running a few miles from home, starting perhaps while we are at work or picking the kids up from school; maybe disability or cost gets in the way. This is where online | + | The antidote |
- | That isn’t | + | The Buddha was arguably the first person |
- | For further reading, see Krusche et al. 2012. For mindfulness online, a preliminary evaluation | + | Mindfulness practices were first adapted for a Western audience around the 1970s. Most significantly, in 1979 Jon Kabat-Zinn |
- | </ | + | |
- | </ | + | |
- | <WRAP box> | + | |
- | Mindfulness Online | + | |
- | < | + | |
- | Adele Krusche and J. Mark G. Willaims, Oxford Mindfulness Centre, University of Oxford | + | |
- | < | + | |
- | Computing is not about computers any more. It is about living. | + | |
- | < | + | |
- | </ | + | |
- | At the Oxford Mindfulness Centre, we teach people | + | |
- | The society we live in and mindfulness therapy seem to be poles apart. A simple concept such as staying still and noticing seems, to many, very alien now, and perhaps that means we need to remember it. Mindfulness | + | Based on MBSR, researchers Zindel Segal, John Teasdale, and J. Mark Williams, director of the Mindfulness |
- | The problem comes when we have classes | + | Privy to the findings |
- | That isn’t to say that this online mindfulness therapy is for everyone. We’re only just starting to explore its effectiveness. Our preliminary research suggests that Internet-based | + | The scientific evidence |
- | For further reading, see Krusche et al. 2012. For mindfulness online, a preliminary evaluation | + | ===== The Psychology |
- | </ | + | ==== Awareness |
- | </ | + | |
+ | Prominent Zen teacher and author Thich Nhat Hanh (2008) describes mindfulness as " | ||
+ | Awareness is the component that continuously monitors the inner and outer world. It is what feeds that which Daniel Kahneman (2013) has called " | ||
+ | |||
+ | ==== Mindfulness as Nonjudgmental Attention ==== | ||
+ | |||
+ | Mindfulness is distinct from the retrospective reflective processes discussed in the previous chapter in at least two ways. First, rather than being a mental account or an analysis of experience, mindfulness is a nonreflective and nonjudgmental observation of it. Mindfulness practice specifically avoids evaluation, opinion construction, | ||
+ | |||
+ | The nonjudgmental, | ||
+ | |||
+ | This relinquishing of judgment is also a key difference between mindfulness and CBT. CBT encourages the labeling of negative thoughts and feelings, for example, as " | ||
+ | |||
+ | ==== Mindfulness: | ||
+ | |||
+ | Brown and Ryan argue that some people may have a disposition (i.e., trait) toward being more mindful, and so they have explored mindfulness as a naturally occurring attribute. Their research has shown that mindfulness as a trait has a positive effect on self-regulated activity and on wellbeing (Brown & Ryan, 2003; Brown, Ryan, & Creswell, 2007). However, they have also studied mindfulness as a state that can occur as a result of training, and their research has found that, independent of disposition, | ||
+ | |||
+ | Research on mindfulness states, the success of programs such as MBSR, and the ancient history of Buddhist practice give ample evidence that mindfulness can be developed with practice, and therefore we can infer that there is potential for technology to be involved in this practice, be it as direct guidance or as peripheral support. But before we get to that, it is worth peeking into the world of the neuroscientist in order to discover how mindfulness physically changes our brain structures and their activity. | ||
+ | |||
+ | ===== The Neuroscience of Mindfulness ===== | ||
+ | |||
+ | Sitting in the confines of his plastic cave, the monk enters a state of deep, spacious awareness. Ready to detect any change in his brainwaves are the 24 electrodes pasted to his conveniently shaven head, and the fMRI machine -- the cave within which he sits -- is sending data on cerebral blood flow to the group of eager scientists behind the glass. Ancient in its origins, meditation is by far the oldest systematic practice for developing mindfulness that exists. Newfangled as mindfulness practice may sound to some, humans were training in it long before they were writing on paper. The practice has survived 2,000 years of human history, and modern-day Buddhist monks continue to engage in and teach these same practices around the globe. Happily, these same monks are also amenable to satisfying the empirical curiosity of neuroscientists, | ||
+ | |||
+ | The physiological impacts of long-term meditation practice are surprisingly conspicuous from a neuroscientific point of view. Although early studies with expert meditators using EEG date back to the 1950s and 1960s, it's only during the past decade that we have been able to accumulate a more rigorous collection of neuroscientific evidence for the impacts of mindfulness practice. | ||
+ | |||
+ | Via EEG studies, mindfulness meditation has been associated with measurable changes to brainwaves, including changes in alpha waves and increases in theta and gamma rhythms (Lutz, Dunne, & Davidson, 2006). Alberto Chiesa and Alessandro Serretti (2010) report that, in addition to significant increases in alpha and theta activity, mindfulness meditation is associated with activation of the prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex, areas related to attention. Moreover, long-term meditation leads to enhancement of both these areas (specifically, | ||
+ | |||
+ | One surprising finding, with implications for those seeking to measure levels of mindfulness physiologically, | ||
+ | |||
+ | ===== Measuring Mindfulness ===== | ||
+ | We can now use EEG and even brain imaging to detect and study mindfulness states, but the simplest, most widely used and thoroughly validated ways to measure mindfulness as both a state and trait are self-report instruments and established mindfulness scales developed by various research groups, each with a slightly different focus. | ||
+ | |||
+ | Brown and Ryan (2003) have provided a theoretical and empirical framework, including a dispositional scale (the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale, or MAAS) that can be used to measure the individual differences in the frequency of mindful states. MAAS consists of 15 statements to which participants assign a score from 1 to 6: for example, "I could be experiencing some emotion and not be conscious about it until some time later" or "I tend to walk quickly to get where I am going without paying attention to what I experience along the way." The questions have been confirmed by a number of studies that have indicated that the MAAS is a valid measure, distinct from other related measures. | ||
+ | |||
+ | The Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory is another widely used scale (Walach, Buchheld, Buttenmuller, | ||
+ | |||
+ | Fortunately, | ||
+ | |||
+ | ==== Measuring the Impact of Mindfulness on Wellbeing ==== | ||
+ | |||
+ | The end goal for any effort in cultivating mindfulness is to foster wellbeing (whether by reducing the experience of pain, preventing depression, treating anxiety, or something else.) In order to measure the link between mindfulness and wellbeing, Brown & Ryan (2003) have used a combination of scales across several studies, including the CES-D Scale and the Beck Depression Inventory (for depression); | ||
+ | |||
+ | These studies showed that mindfulness (as measured by MAAS) was significantly correlated with measures of self-regulation and wellbeing. Furthermore, | ||
+ | |||
+ | Some of the limitations of Brown and Ryan's experience-sampling study (for example, the number of participants, | ||
+ | |||
+ | * How are you feeling right now? (answers were on a sliding 1-100 scale) | ||
+ | * What are you doing right now? (with a choice of 22 activities generally used in the day-reconstruction method (Kahneman, Krueger, Schkade, Schwarz, & Stone, 2004) | ||
+ | * Are you thinking about something other than what you are doing? (with the following options: " | ||
+ | |||
+ | The application collected an impressive 2.5 million samples from 5,000 individuals from 83 different countries ages 18 to 88. From among these samples, they analyzed the US-derived data (N = 2,250), and the findings were striking. | ||
+ | |||
+ | - Mind-wandering occurred about half the time. Their findings suggest that on average 46.5 percent of the time we are thinking about something other than what we're doing. This happens at least 30 percent of the time for specific activities (with the exception of love making, for which we mind wander -- or admit to it -- only about 10 percent of the time). Interestingly, | ||
+ | - People were less happy when their minds wandered, regardless of what they were doing at that time. Minds wandered to positive things 42.5 percent of the time, to negative things 26.5 percent of the time, and to neutral things 31 percent of the time. And here's the clincher: people were not happier when their minds wandered to positive thoughts than when they were simply being mindful of the task at hand (whatever it may be). In other words, fantasizing about a tropical island while sitting in dull traffic will not make you happier than mindfully attending to the traffic. | ||
+ | - What people were thinking was more important to predicting how they felt than what they were doing. The nature of participant activity explained only 4.6 percent of the variance in happiness, while mind wandering explained 10.8 percent. | ||
+ | - Most significantly, | ||
+ | |||
+ | Clearly, mind wandering is a mental activity that sits in opposition to mindfulness in that it describes doing one thing but thinking another. It seems logical then to suspect that multitasking, | ||
+ | |||
+ | In a recent survey of more than 3,000 girls ages 8?12 (Pea et al., 2012), media multitasking was shown to be negatively correlated to wellbeing (media multitasking constituted combinations of watching video, playing videogames, listening to music, texting, and talking over the phone). Specifically, | ||
+ | |||
+ | Multitasking research is particularly relevant to positive computing because the current design of technology actively facilitates parallel activity. This is probably because most digital devices are considered productivity tools, and multitasking is culturally viewed largely as a positive expression of greater productivity, | ||
+ | |||
+ | Digitally afforded multitasking trains us to be drawn away from what we're experiencing and to be distractible. Whenever we come across a few seconds of pause (e.g., a file download, a line at the grocery store), we have come to react by immediately seeking something to fill in the moment, and mobile phones make this possible anywhere, anytime. Our brains seem addicted to busyness, despite our pleas for rest. Time and again we react to the prospect of present-moment awareness with anxiety, rapidly seeking out new points of attention -- we even look for new thoughts to think (What can I plan? I should check my schedule mentally while I stand here) rather than settling into the moment as it is. Ironically, this is all at the expense of both performance and wellbeing. | ||
+ | |||
+ | With multiple devices on hand, each of which houses multiple applications displayed in parallel windows, modern devices are multitasking dynamos. It is only recently in response to information overload that some software designs have been switching some of the focus back to well … focus. If multitasking does in fact decrease wellbeing, researchers in positive computing need to investigate how we can support productivity (genuinely) and in ways that don't compromise wellbeing. | ||
+ | |||
+ | The abundance of research evidence for the wellbeing benefits of mindfulness gives us plenty of reason to support it in the context of positive computing. However, how do we approach supporting users with a factor so subjective and internally experienced? | ||
+ | |||
+ | ===== Strategies and Interventions for Fostering Mindfulness ===== | ||
+ | ==== Mindfulness Training and Meditation ==== | ||
+ | |||
+ | Jon Kabat-Zinn' | ||
+ | |||
+ | Mindfulness-based training programs and mindfulness-based CBT programs incorporate multiple strategies, including mindfulness meditation. For mindfulness-based meditation (MBM in the scientific literature), | ||
+ | |||
+ | The core of mindfulness meditation involves training the mind to reel itself back in whenever it wanders, as it wanders, over and over again. As Pema Chodron (2007) puts it, " | ||
+ | |||
+ | Although most modern technology trains us in splitting attention, there are a number of initiatives, | ||
+ | |||
+ | ==== Strategies in Education ==== | ||
+ | |||
+ | Despite the fact that William James had already espoused the value of mindfulness training for education back in the nineteenth century, we are only now beginning to see this training find its way into the curriculum. Politicians, | ||
+ | |||
+ | Tim Ryan (2012), a Democratic congressman from Ohio, has shared his views on the ways in which mindfulness can change schooling, the health-care system, the military, and even the nation. His basic tenet is that mindfulness is an important aspect of the socioemotional skill set that leads to good conflict resolution, responsible decision making, better relationships, | ||
+ | |||
+ | The Association for Mindfulness in Education, a collection of organizations and individuals promoting the introduction of mindfulness practices in education maintains a list of schools and programs that already include mindfulness in their curriculum.4 | ||
+ | |||
+ | Integrating mindfulness training programs or other mindfulness interventions into people' | ||
+ | |||
+ | ==== Biofeedback Interventions ==== | ||
+ | |||
+ | As mentioned earlier, there are measurable correlations between mindfulness states and various physiological signals (Chiesa & Serretti, 2010; Lutz et al., 2006), and although these signals are useful for research, some researchers have gone further to ask how they might be used as feedback for mindfulness training. Biofeedback systems record physiological signals and feed them back to the user as sound or visual stimuli in real time. The signals may include a variety of inputs from EEG and electrocardiogram to heart rate or breathing patterns. By receiving this synced real-time feedback, a participant can learn to change a detected physiological factor, for example, via operant conditioning (with a subtle reward system). | ||
+ | |||
+ | There is some minimal early evidence that biofeedback systems could be helpful in supporting mindfulness or at least some of the characteristics associated with mindfulness, | ||
+ | |||
+ | Although the price, size, and accessibility of biofeedback sensors and systems have been plummeting over the past few years, most experimental systems remain fairly intrusive or expensive or both. An alternative approach comprises a challenge to positive computing: How can mindfulness states and training be effectively supported by common digital technologies? | ||
+ | |||
+ | ===== Digital Technology for Mindfulness ===== | ||
+ | From melting into an embodied symphonic experience to following the reassuring guidance of a soft voice recording or even sharing your efforts with friends, the examples of technology-mediated support for mindfulness, | ||
+ | |||
+ | ==== Guided Sessions ==== | ||
+ | |||
+ | |||
+ | Smiling Mind is a collection of mindfulness-training activities delivered over the Internet and via mobile app. Like other audio-instruction-based courses, it provides a series of guided meditation recordings. It differentiates itself, however, by providing these recordings in the context of tailored training curricula. Smiling Mind provides content targeted to various contexts (e.g., a curriculum program aimed at school-age children and a corporate training program for adults). For each of the programs, the focus has been on developing a high-quality curriculum and set of materials. | ||
+ | |||
+ | Other examples of guided support have specifically targeted stress in the workplace. Mark Williams and his group at the University of Oxford (Krusche, Cyhlarova, King, & Williams, 2012) evaluated a set of modules from the MBSR and MBCT programs delivered over six weeks. The Perceived Stress Scale (Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, | ||
+ | |||
+ | ==== Social Features in Mindfulness Training ==== | ||
+ | |||
+ | |||
+ | Projects such as Mindfulnets5 distinguish themselves by adding social networking features that allow users to share their ongoing experience with others. The user fills out two questionnaires, | ||
+ | |||
+ | An interesting feature of Mindfulnets is its integration with Facebook. Users are able to comment on their progress, and discussions are visible to visitors. Another feature is a page displaying personal statistics and results of the Perceived Stress Scale and the Five Facets Mindfulness Questionnaire. These sharing features could hypothetically have two types of impact. On the one hand, the sharing of experience may increase motivation. On the other hand, users may be more likely to compare themselves to others and judge themselves, which is recognized as a disabling practice for mindfulness. The website intervention was evaluated (Quintana & Rivera, 2012) giving small effect sizes, albeit with a number of disclaimers due to the small and sparse distribution of the subjects' | ||
+ | |||
+ | One study (Morledge et al., 2013) evaluated a mindfulness intervention for which a content-centric program was compared to the same intervention but combined with discussion forums. The results reproduced the positive effects of mindfulness intervention shown in other studies, with statistically significant improvements in perceived stress and mindfulness for both the content-based version and the version with social features at 8 and 12 weeks. With regard to the social features, the authors stated, "Some evidence from our study suggested that this component improved some participants' | ||
+ | |||
+ | More research is currently needed before we can understand if social interaction and sharing are beneficial to mindfulness practice and interventions, | ||
+ | |||
+ | ==== Embodied Experience ==== | ||
+ | |||
+ | |||
+ | Very few have explored embodied experiences for supporting mindfulness, | ||
+ | |||
+ | For a more thoroughly embodied experience, we turn to Sonic Cradle. In the Sonic Cradle, you are suspended in a hammock-like " | ||
+ | |||
+ | But mindful awareness is by no means restricted to the meditative context and is intended as a way of life. Buddhist monks practice mindful walking, eating, cleaning, and living in general. Mindful eating is gaining popularity in the mainstream, and programs have even found their way into corporate offices (Google recently invited Zen teacher Thich Nhat Hanh to come and give their employees guidance on mindful eating). | ||
+ | |||
+ | A few technologies already tackle mindfulness support in day-to-day activity. For example, the Breath -- Walk Aware System was designed to support beginning meditators in the practice of walking meditation using a smartphone (Yu, Wu, Lee, & Hung, 2012). Similarly, the Slow Floor employs pressure-sensitive surfaces and sound to promote awareness of bodily movement (Feltham & Loke, 2012). | ||
+ | |||
+ | The HAPIfork is a commercial product that intervenes at mealtimes by vibrating when you eat too fast. The electronic fork promises to promote mindful eating (and associated weight loss) and is also a data-collection tool that records the time it took you to finish a meal and the speed at which you ate (measured by "fork servings" | ||
+ | |||
+ | From light-up floors to shaking forks, we're clearly only seeing the very tip of the iceberg when it comes to how embodiment might be employed to support mindful attention during meditation and daily activity. It will be a while yet before we can come to know what is genuinely effective and what is obstructive or merely superfluous to such endeavors, and we can expect there to be differences across contexts, levels of experience, and possibly cultures. At any rate, embodiment research will be an interesting space to watch (and sample) as future developments arise. | ||
+ | |||
+ | ==== Games for Mindfulness? | ||
+ | |||
+ | |||
+ | Although there are websites eager to suggest that common videogames train us in mindfulness, | ||
+ | |||
+ | Games+Learning+Society (GLS), a group of researchers and developers based in Madison, Wisconsin, is working on games specifically designed to provide practice in aspects of mindfulness and other wellbeing-related skills. One of these games, called Tenacity, requires the player to tap an iPad once for each breath and twice for each fifth breath through increasing levels of distracting difficulty. Researchers at GLS, who include neuroscientists such as Richard Davidson, study the psychological effects of videogames using a combination of behavioral, structural, and fMRI-based measures -- an approach that is sure to reveal important insights for positive computing designers. | ||
+ | |||
+ | ===== Design Implications ===== | ||
+ | |||
+ | ==== Distraction versus Guidance ==== | ||
+ | |||
+ | In Japanese monasteries, | ||
+ | |||
+ | First, in the case of autonomy, whether the stick is an affront or an invigorating aid has something to do with whether you agreed to it or not. For software design, that means honoring user agency as to when, where, and how help occurs. Moreover, autonomy issues can extend into important details. Can the user set limits, apply variations, or otherwise take part in shaping the support environment or service? After all, it's ultimately a kind of improved autonomy that one is attempting to foster with mindfulness training, in the form of improved self-regulation of attention. | ||
+ | |||
+ | Second, there is reason to believe that minimalism is critical to design intended to support mindfulness. We have already discussed how modern digital environments, | ||
+ | |||
+ | By minimalism we also mean subtlety. You might say the Zen master' | ||
+ | |||
+ | ==== Using Aural and Haptic Feedback ==== | ||
+ | |||
+ | |||
+ | The bell and stick go back centuries as effective tools for supporting mindfulness and meditation. In modern terms, they might be described as tools for aural and haptic feedback. Another form of haptic feedback is seen in a common meditation posture that requires holding the hands in the lap with thumbs almost touching (the "Zen mudra" | ||
+ | |||
+ | Some of the modern examples mentioned previously, such as Sonic Cradle, have successfully employed aural feedback to represent physiological signals. In another example, the Tibetan " | ||
+ | |||
+ | ==== Supporting Nonjudgment ==== | ||
+ | |||
+ | |||
+ | We have already alluded to the potential pitfalls of applying certain motivational features such as tracking and goal-setting to meditative practice and mindfulness training, although they are highly effective in other contexts. We also highlighted the " | ||
+ | |||
+ | Jon Kabat-Zinn (2003) explains that in MBSR clients are encouraged "to let go of their expectations, | ||
+ | |||
+ | Yet how do you reconcile the need for purpose and direction in lifestyle change as well as people' | ||
+ | |||
+ | In other words, an intention may be "I focus today on returning to the present moment each time I realize my mind has wandered" | ||
+ | |||
+ | Support for self-compassion may also prove helpful in balancing out our tendency to strive and judge in this context. Kabat-Zinn (2003) suggests the answer to reconciling nonstriving with the reality of valid intentions is rooted in teacher authenticity, | ||
+ | |||
+ | ==== Practicing What You Teach (or Design For) ==== | ||
+ | |||
+ | |||
+ | Although there do not seem to be any downsides to mindfulness (for example, there are no reported negative side effects), Kabat-Zinn has drawn attention to the potential pitfalls that scientists (and, we infer, technologists) are at risk of encountering as interest in mindfulness gains momentum within clinical practice and beyond. In a thorough commentary, Kabat-Zinn (2003) cautions: | ||
+ | |||
+ | It becomes critically important that those persons coming to the field with professional interest and enthusiasm recognize the unique qualities and characteristics of mindfulness as a meditative practice, with all that implies, so that mindfulness is not simply seized upon as the next promising cognitive behavioral technique or exercise, decontextualized, | ||
+ | |||
+ | Specifically, | ||
+ | |||
+ | If we fail to practice first, either because we are too busy or not sufficiently interested, Kabat-Zinn (2003) warns that our " | ||
+ | |||
+ | In the case of technology, we can just as easily get carried away by enthusiasm, by our desire to fit a mindfulness agenda into a particular technology or vice versa, or by a temptation to compromise critical principles of mindfulness for the sake of quantifiable goals or aesthetic values. The lesson is simple: when it comes to mindfulness, | ||
+ | |||
+ | |||
+ | ==== Notes ==== | ||
+ | |||
+ | 1. The eight steps are: Right View, Right Intentions, Right Speech, Right Actions, Right Livelihood, Right Effort, Right Mindfulness, | ||
+ | 2. The research guide can be found at mindfulexperience.org. | ||
+ | 3. This means that the relationship between the intervention (doing MBSR) and wellbeing is considered high. | ||
+ | 4. For this promotion, see the website mindfuleducation.org. | ||
+ | 5. See the website mindfulnets.co. | ||
+ | 6. Although the full experience requires the cradle and a sound-proof room, you can get a taste of it at jayvidyarthi.com. | ||
+ | |||
+ | ==== References ==== | ||
+ | |||
+ | * Baer, R. A. (2011). Measuring mindfulness. Contemporary Buddhism, 12(1), 241-261. | ||
+ | * Becker, M. W., Alzahabi, R., & Hopwood, C. J. (2013). Media multitasking is associated with symptoms of depression and social anxiety. Cyberpsychology, | ||
+ | * Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822-848. | ||
+ | * Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., & Creswell, J. D. (2007). Mindfulness: | ||
+ | * Chiesa, A., & Serretti, A. (2010). A systematic review of neurobiological and clinical features of mindfulness meditations. Psychological Medicine, 40(8), 1239-1252. | ||
+ | * Chodron, P. (2007). How to meditate with Pema Chodron: A practical guide to making friends with your mind. Boulder, CO: Sounds True. | ||
+ | * Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, | ||
+ | * Feltham, F., & Loke, L. (2012). The slow floor: Towards an awareness of bodily movement through interactive walking surfaces. Paper presented at the workshop "The Body in Design," | ||
+ | * Grossman, P., Niemann, L., Schmidt, S., & Walach, H. (2004). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health benefits. A meta-analysis. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 57(1), 35-43. | ||
+ | * Hamilton, N. A., Kitzman, H., & Guyotte, S. (2006). Enhancing health and emotion: Mindfulness as a missing link between cognitive therapy and positive psychology. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, | ||
+ | * Hanh, T. N. (2008). The miracle of mindfulness. New York: Random House. | ||
+ | * James, W. (1892). Psychology: A briefer course. New York: Henry Holt. | ||
+ | * Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your body and mind to face stress, pain, and illness. New York: Delacourt. | ||
+ | * Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present, and future. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10(2), 144-156. | ||
+ | * Kahneman, D. (2013). Thinking, fast and slow (p. 512). New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. | ||
+ | * Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. B., Schkade, D. A., Schwarz, N. S., & Stone, A. A. (2004). A survey method for characterizing daily life experience: The day reconstruction method. Science, 306(5702), 1776-1780. | ||
+ | * Khalsa, S. S., Rudrauf, D., Damasio, A. R., Davidson, R. J., Lutz, A., & Tranel, D. (2008). Interoceptive awareness in experienced meditators. Psychophysiology, | ||
+ | * Killingsworth, | ||
+ | * Krusche, A., Cyhlarova, E., King, S., & Williams, J. M. G. (2012). Mindfulness online: A preliminary evaluation of the feasibility of a web-based mindfulness course and the impact on stress. BMJ Open, 2(3). Retrieved from http:// | ||
+ | * Lutz, A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2006). Meditation and the neuroscience of consciousness: | ||
+ | * Morledge, T. J., Allexandre, D., Fox, E., Fu, A. Z., Higashi, M. K., Kruzikas, D. T., … Reese, P. R. (2013). Feasibility of an online mindfulness program for stress management: A randomized, controlled trial. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 46(2): | ||
+ | * Negroponte, N. 1995. Being digital. New York: Vintage. | ||
+ | * Pea, R., Nass, C., Meheula, L., Rance, M., Kumar, A., Bamford, H., … Zhou, M. (2012). Media use, face-to-face communication, | ||
+ | * Plasier, S., Bulut, M., & Aarts, R. (2011). A study of monaural beat effects on brain activity using an electronic singing bowl. Annual Symposium of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBS). Benelux Chapter, December 2 (Brussels/ | ||
+ | * Quintana, M., & Rivera, O. (2012). Mindfulness training online for stress reduction, a global measure. Studies in Health Technology and Informatics, | ||
+ | * Ryan, T. (2012). A mindful nation. Carlsbad, CA: Hay House. | ||
+ | * Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, J. D. (2012). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression (p. 471). New York: Guilford Press. | ||
+ | * Speca, M., Carlson, L. E., Goodey, E., & Angen, M. (2000). A randomized, wait-list controlled clinical trial: The effect of a mindfulness meditation-based stress reduction program on mood and symptoms of stress in cancer outpatients. Psychosomatic Medicine, 62(5), 613-622. | ||
+ | * Stinson, B., & Arthur, D. (2013). A novel EEG for alpha brain state training, neurobiofeedback, | ||
+ | * Vidyarthi, J., & Riecke, B. (2013). Mediated meditation: Cultivating mindfulness with sonic cradle. In CHI13 extended abstracts on human factors in computing systems (pp. 2305-2314). New York: ACM. | ||
+ | * Walach, H., Buchheld, N., Buttenmuller, | ||
+ | * Wang, Z., & Tchernev, J. M. (2012). The " | ||
+ | * Williams, J. M. G., & Kabat-Zinn, J. (Eds.). (2013). Mindfulness: | ||
+ | * Yu, M.-C., Wu, H., Lee, M.-S., & Hung, Y.-P. (2012). Multimedia-assisted breathwalk-aware system. IEEE Transactions on Bio-medical Engineering, | ||
+ | |||
+ | </ | ||
+ | .... | ||
+ | </ |
book/positive_computing/9_mindfulness.1468246679.txt.gz · Last modified: 2016/07/11 22:47 by hkimscil